雅思阅读第120套P3-The creation of lasting memories

雅思阅读第120套P3-The creation of lasting memories-托您的福
雅思阅读第120套P3-The creation of lasting memories
此内容为付费阅读,请付费后查看
3
限时特惠
9
您当前未登录!建议登陆后购买,可保存购买订单
付费阅读
已售 12

The creation of lasting memories

Many studies of the brain processes underlying the creation of memory consolidation (lasting memories) have involved giving various human and animal subjects treatment, while training them to perform a task. These have contributed greatly to our understanding.

In pioneering studies using goldfish, Bernard Agranoff found that protein synthesis inhibitors injected after training caused the goldfish to forget what they had learned. In other experiments, he administered protein synthesis inhibitors immediately before the fish were trained. The remarkable finding was that the fish learned the task completely normally, but forgot it within a few hours – that is, the protein synthesis inhibitors blocked memory consolidation, but did not influence short-term memory.

There is now extensive evidence that short-term memory is spared by many kinds of treatments, including electro-convulsive therapy (ECT), that block memory consolidation. On the other hand, and equally importantly, neuroscientist Ivan Izquierdo found that many drug treatments can block short-term memory without blocking memory consolidation. Contrary to the hypothesis put forward by Canadian psychologist Donald Hebb, in 1949, long-term memory does not require short-term memory, and vice versa.

Such findings suggest that our experiences create parallel, and possibly independent stages of memory, .each with a different life span. All of this evidence from clinical and experimental studies strongly indicates that the brain handles recent and remote memory in different ways; but why does it do that?

We obviously need to have memory that is created rapidly: reacting to an ever and rapidly changing environment requires that. For example, most current building codes require that the heights of all steps in a staircase be equal. After taking a couple of steps, up or down, we implicitly remember the heights of the steps and assume that the others will be the same. If they are not the same, we are very likely to trip and fall. Lack of this kind of rapidly created implicit memory would be bad for us and for insurance companies, but perhaps good for lawyers. It would be of little value to us if we remembered the heights of the steps only after a delay of many hours, when the memory becomes consolidated.

The hypothesis that lasting memory consolidates slowly over time is supported primarily by clinical and experimental evidence that the formation of long-term memory is influenced by treatments and disorders affecting brain functioning. There are also other kinds of evidence indicating more directly that the memories consolidate over time after learning. Avi Kami and Dov Sagi reported that the performance of human subjects trained in a visual skill did not improve until eight hours after the training was completed, and that improvement was even greater the following day. Furthermore, the skill was retained for several years.

Studies using human brain imaging to study changes in neural activity induced by learning have also reported that the changes continue to develop for hours after learning. In an innovative study using functional imaging of the brain, Reza Shadmehr and Henry Holcomb examined brain activity in several brain regions shortly after human subjects were trained in a motor learning task requiring arm and hand movements. They found that while the performance of the subjects remained stable for several hours after completion of the training, their brain activity did not; different regions of the brain were predominantly active at different times over a period of several hours after the training. The activity shifted from the prefrontal cortex to two areas known to be involved in controlling movements, the motor cortex and cerebellar cortex. Consolidation of the motor skill appeared to involve activation of different neural systems that increased the stability of the brain processes underlying the skill.

There is also evidence that learning-induced changes in the activity of neurons in the cerebral cortex continue to increase for many days after the training. In an extensive series of studies using rats with electrodes implanted in the auditory cortex, Norman Weinberger reported that, after a tone of specific frequency was paired a few times with footshock, neurons in the rats’ auditory cortex responded more to that specific tone and less to other tones of other frequencies. Even more interestingly, the selectivity of the neurons’ response to the specific tone used in training continued to increase for several days after the training was terminated.

It is not intuitively obvious why our lasting memories consolidate slowly. Certainly, one can wonder why we have a form of memory that we have to rely on for many hours, days or a lifetime, that is so susceptible to disruption shortly after it is initiated. Perhaps the brain system that consolidates long-term memory over time was a late development in vertebrate evolution. Moreover, maybe we consolidate memories slowly because our mammalian brains are large and enormously complex. We can readily reject these ideas. All species of animals studied to date have both short and long-term memory; and all are susceptible to retrograde amnesia. Like humans, birds, bees, and molluscs, as well as fish and rats, make long-term memory slowly. Consolidation of memory clearly emerged early in evolution, and was conserved.

Although there seems to be no compelling reason to conclude that a biological system such as a brain could not quickly make a lasting memory, the fact is that animal brains do not. Thus, memory consolidation must serve some very important adaptive function or functions. There is considerable evidence suggesting that the slow consolidation is adaptive because it enables neurobiological processes occurring shortly after learning to influence the strength of memory for experiences. The extensive evidence that memory can be enhanced, as well as impaired, by treatments administered shortly after training, provides intriguing support for this hypothesis.

 

SECTION 3: QUESTIONS 27-40

Questions 27-31

Choose the correct letter, A, B, C or D.

 

27Experiments by Bernard Agranoff described in Reading Passage 3 involved

Ainjecting goldfish at different stages of the experiments.

Btraining goldfish to do different types of task.

Cusing different types of treatment on goldfish.

Dcomparing the performance of different goldfish on certain tasks.

 

28Most findings from recent studies suggest that

Adrug treatments do not normally affect short-term memories.

Blong-term memories build upon short-term memories.

Cshort and long-term memories are formed by separate processes.

DECT treatment affects both short-and long-term memories.

 

29In the fifth paragraph, what does the writer want to show by the example of staircases?

APrompt memory formation underlies the performance of everyday tasks.

BRoutine tasks can be carried out unconsciously.

CPhysical accidents can impair the function of memory.

DComplex information such as regulations cannot be retained by the memory.

 

30Observations about memory by Kami and Sagi

Acast doubt on existing hypotheses.

Brelated only to short-term memory.

Cwere based on tasks involving hearing.

Dconfirmed other experimental findings.

 

31What did the experiment by Shadmehr and Holcomb show?

ADifferent areas of the brain were activated by different tasks.

BActivity in the brain gradually moved from one area to other areas.

CSubjects continued to get better at a task after training had finished.

DTreatment given to subjects improved their performance on a task.

Questions 32-36

Do the following statements agree with the views of the writer in Reading Passage 3?

Write

 

YES

if the statement agrees with the views of the writer

NO

if the statement contradicts the views of the writer

NOT GIVEN

if it is impossible to say what the writer thinks about this

 

32 _________________ The training which Kami and Sagi’s subjects were given was repeated over several days.

33 _________________ The rats in Weinberger’s studies learned to associate a certain sound with a specific experience.

34 _________________ The results of Weinberger’s studies indicated that the strength of the rats’ learned associations increases with time.

35 _________________ It is easy to see the evolutionary advantage of the way lasting memories in humans are created.

36 _________________ Long-term memories in humans are more stable than in many other species.

Questions 37-40

Complete the summary using the list of words, A-l, below

Long-term memory

Various researchers have examined the way lasting memories are formed. Laboratory experiments usually involve teaching subjects to do something 37 _________________ and treating them with mild electric shocks or drugs. Other studies monitor behaviour after a learning experience, or use sophisticated equipment to observe brain activity.

The results are generally consistent: they show that lasting memories are the result of a 38 _________________ and complex biological process.

The fact that humans share this trait with other species, including animals with 39 _________________ brains, suggests that it developed 40 _________________ in our evolutionary history.

 

A

early

B

easy

C

large

D

late

E

lengthy

F

new

G

recently

H

small

I

quick

 

答案请付费后查阅:

 

© 版权声明
THE END
喜欢就支持一下吧
点赞1
分享
评论 抢沙发
tuonindefu的头像-托您的福

昵称

取消
昵称表情代码图片